Educational level and cognitive impairment in patients with Parkinson disease

8
Neurologia i Neurochirurgia Polska 2011; 45, 1 24 Correspondence address: dr Anna Kierzynka, Klinika Neurologii, Uniwersytet Medyczny w Poznaniu, ul. Przybyszewskiego 49, 60-335 Poznañ, e-mail: [email protected] Received: 18.02.2010; accepted: 26.10.2010 Abstract Background and purpose: Parkinson disease (PD) is a risk factor for dementia. In addition, specific cognitive deficits can occur in PD patients without dementia. A patient’s lev- el of education could have an influence on the development of cognitive impairment in PD. The aim of this study was to examine the relationship between the level of education and cognitive performance in non-demented patients with PD. Material and methods: Thirty-seven consecutive, nonde- mented PD patients and 40 healthy controls fulfilled the inclusion criteria and were enrolled in the case-control study. Each of the controls and PD patients were classified, for the purpose of this study, into one of three groups (low, interme- diate, higher), categorized by the number of years of educa- tion. There were no differences in education and age between the controls and PD patients. All of the subjects were evalu- ated with a battery of neuropsychological tests: Mini-Men- tal State Examination, Trail Making Tests, Stroop Test, Men- tal Rotation Test, and Verbal Fluency Test. Results: Less (low and intermediate) education was corre- lated with poor results from tests. The comparison of all groups of PD patients and controls demonstrated that PD subjects received lower test scores, especially for the low and intermediate groups. However, no statistically significant dif- ference was reached between educationally advanced PD patients and the appropriate control subjects. Conclusions: As compared to the controls, most non-dement- ed PD patients presented executive-type cognitive dysfunc- tion. The higher educational level, however, was associated Educational level and cognitive impairment in patients with Parkinson disease Wykszta³cenie a zaburzenia funkcji poznawczych u osób z chorob¹ Parkinsona Anna Kierzynka 1 , Rados³aw KaŸmierski 2 , Wojciech Kozubski 1 1 Katedra i Klinika Neurologii, Uniwersytet Medyczny w Poznaniu, Szpital im. H. Œwiêcickiego w Poznaniu 2 Klinika Neurologii i Chorób Naczyniowych Uk³adu Nerwowego, Uniwersytet Medyczny w Poznaniu, Szpital MSWiA im. prof. L. Bierkowskiego w Poznaniu Neurologia i Neurochirurgia Polska 2011; 45, 1: 24–31 ORIGINAL PAPER/ARTYKU£ ORYGINALNY Streszczenie Wstêp i cel pracy: Choroba Parkinsona (ChP) jest czynni- kiem ryzyka wyst¹pienia otêpienia, pewne zaburzenia poznaw- cze mog¹ wystêpowaæ jednak¿e ju¿ u chorych niespe³niaj¹cych kryteriów demencji. Poziom wykszta³cenia mo¿e mieæ wp³yw na obecnoœæ zaburzeñ poznawczych. Postuluje siê, ¿e niski poziom edukacji mo¿e byæ zwi¹zany z szybszym pogorsze- niem funkcjonowania poznawczego, a wy¿sze wykszta³cenie mo¿e stanowiæ czynnik ochronny. Celem badañ by³a próba ustalenia zwi¹zku pomiêdzy obecnoœci¹ zaburzeñ poznaw- czych a poziomem wykszta³cenia u chorych na ChP bez otêpienia. Materia³ i metody: Przebadano 37 chorych na ChP i 40 osób z grupy kontrolnej. Dla celów pracy badanych podzielono na trzy podgrupy w zale¿noœci od poziomu wykszta³cenia (pod- stawowe/zawodowe, œrednie, wy¿sze). Poszczególne podgru- py osób badanych i kontrolnych nie ró¿ni³y siê pod wzglêdem liczebnoœci i wieku. W obu grupach oceniono funkcjonowa- nie poznawcze przy zastosowaniu testów psychologicznych (Krótka Skala Oceny Stanu Psychicznego, Test £¹czenia Punktów, Test Stroopa, Test Rotacji Figury, Test Fluencji S³ownej). Wyniki: Ni¿sze wykszta³cenie (podstawowe/zawodowe i œred- nie) wi¹za³o siê z gorszymi wynikami testów psychologicz- nych. Porównanie grupy chorych i kontrolnej wykaza³o, i¿ chorzy osi¹gaj¹ gorsze wyniki testów, w szczególnoœci chorzy z ni¿szym wykszta³ceniem (podstawowym/zawodowym i œred- nim) w porównaniu z osobami z grupy kontrolnej o tym samym poziomie wykszta³cenia. Porównuj¹c chorych na ChP

Transcript of Educational level and cognitive impairment in patients with Parkinson disease

Page 1: Educational level and cognitive impairment in patients with Parkinson disease

NNeeuurroollooggiiaa ii NNeeuurroocchhiirruurrggiiaa PPoollsskkaa 2011; 45, 124

Correspondence address: dr Anna Kierzynka, Klinika Neurologii, Uniwersytet Medyczny w Poznaniu, ul. Przybyszewskiego 49, 60-335 Poznañ,e-mail: [email protected] Received: 18.02.2010; accepted: 26.10.2010

AAbbss ttrraacctt

BBaacckkggrroouunndd aanndd ppuurrppoossee:: Parkinson disease (PD) is a riskfactor for dementia. In addition, specific cognitive deficitscan occur in PD patients without dementia. A patient’s lev-el of education could have an influence on the developmentof cognitive impairment in PD. The aim of this study was toexamine the relationship between the level of education andcognitive performance in non-demented patients with PD.MMaatteerriiaall aanndd mmeetthhooddss:: Thirty-seven consecutive, nonde-mented PD patients and 40 healthy controls fulfilled theinclusion criteria and were enrolled in the case-control study.Each of the controls and PD patients were classified, for thepurpose of this study, into one of three groups (low, interme-diate, higher), categorized by the number of years of educa-tion. There were no differences in education and age betweenthe controls and PD patients. All of the subjects were evalu-ated with a battery of neuropsychological tests: Mini-Men-tal State Examination, Trail Making Tests, Stroop Test, Men-tal Rotation Test, and Verbal Fluency Test.RReessuullttss:: Less (low and intermediate) education was corre-lated with poor results from tests. The comparison of allgroups of PD patients and controls demonstrated that PDsubjects received lower test scores, especially for the low andintermediate groups. However, no statistically significant dif-ference was reached between educationally advanced PDpatients and the appropriate control subjects.CCoonncclluussiioonnss:: As compared to the controls, most non-dement-ed PD patients presented executive-type cognitive dysfunc-tion. The higher educational level, however, was associated

Educational level and cognitive impairment in patients with Parkinson diseaseWykszta³cenie a zaburzenia funkcji poznawczych u osób z chorob¹ Parkinsona

Anna Kierzynka1, Rados³aw KaŸmierski2, Wojciech Kozubski1

1Katedra i Klinika Neurologii, Uniwersytet Medyczny w Poznaniu, Szpital im. H. Œwiêcickiego w Poznaniu2Klinika Neurologii i Chorób Naczyniowych Uk³adu Nerwowego, Uniwersytet Medyczny w Poznaniu, Szpital MSWiA im. prof. L. Bierkowskiego w Poznaniu

Neurologia i Neurochirurgia Polska 2011; 45, 1: 24–31

ORIGINAL PAPER/ARTYKU£ ORYGINALNY

SStt rreesszzcc zzeenn iiee

WWssttêêpp ii cceell pprraaccyy:: Choroba Parkinsona (ChP) jest czynni-kiem ryzyka wyst¹pienia otêpienia, pewne zaburzenia poznaw-cze mog¹ wystêpowaæ jednak¿e ju¿ u chorych niespe³ niaj¹cychkryteriów demencji. Poziom wykszta³cenia mo¿e mieæ wp³ywna obecnoœæ zaburzeñ poznawczych. Postuluje siê, ¿e niskipoziom edukacji mo¿e byæ zwi¹zany z szybszym pogorsze-niem funkcjonowania poznawczego, a wy¿sze wykszta³ceniemo¿e stanowiæ czynnik ochronny. Celem badañ by³a próbaustalenia zwi¹zku pomiêdzy obecnoœci¹ zaburzeñ poznaw-czych a poziomem wykszta³cenia u chorych na ChP bezotêpienia.MMaatteerriiaa³³ ii mmeettooddyy:: Przebadano 37 chorych na ChP i 40 osóbz grupy kontrolnej. Dla celów pracy badanych podzielono natrzy podgrupy w zale¿noœci od poziomu wykszta³cenia (pod-stawowe/zawodowe, œrednie, wy¿sze). Poszczególne podgru-py osób badanych i kontrolnych nie ró¿ni³y siê pod wzglêdemliczebnoœci i wieku. W obu grupach oceniono funkcjonowa-nie poznawcze przy zastosowaniu testów psychologicznych(Krótka Skala Oceny Stanu Psychicznego, Test £¹czeniaPunktów, Test Stroopa, Test Rotacji Figury, Test FluencjiS³ownej).WWyynniikkii:: Ni¿sze wykszta³cenie (podstawowe/zawodowe i œred-nie) wi¹za³o siê z gorszymi wynikami testów psychologicz-nych. Porównanie grupy chorych i kontrolnej wykaza³o, i¿chorzy osi¹gaj¹ gorsze wyniki testów, w szczególnoœci chorzyz ni¿szym wykszta³ceniem (podstawowym/zawodowym i œred-nim) w porównaniu z osobami z grupy kontrolnej o tymsamym poziomie wykszta³cenia. Porównuj¹c chorych na ChP

Page 2: Educational level and cognitive impairment in patients with Parkinson disease

NNeeuurroollooggiiaa ii NNeeuurroocchhiirruurrggiiaa PPoollsskkaa 2011; 45, 1 25

IInnttrroodduuccttiioonn

Cognitive deficits are common in Parkinson disease(PD), even in early stages and in newly diagnosed cases [1-4]. In many patients, these deficits may not beclinically apparent, but they are detectable with specificneuropsychological tests. According to Caviness et al.,neuropsychological testing for abnormalities in non-demented patients could have predictive value for thepotential development of dementia [5]. The most fre-quent early cognitive abnormalities in PD are executiveand visuospatial dysfunction, attention and memoryimpairment, and bradyphrenia [6-9].

The prevalence of dementia in PD is estimated ata level of 24 to 31% [10,11], and longitudinal studiesshow a frequency as high as 60% after 12 years of fol-low-up [12] and even 83% in 20-year survivors [13].With such high rates of cognitive decline, diagnosticmethods and tests that could help in identifying PDpatients at higher risk of developing dementia couldbe useful in planning patient management and treat-ment.

A number of risk factors for cognitive deteriorationand dementia in PD patients have been identified,including the following: older age [11,12,14,15], laterage at onset [4,14], longer duration of PD [12], moreadvanced motor symptoms [15] and bradykinesia- dominant PD with postural instability and gait difficulty[8,15,16].

Recently, the positive role of education has beendescribed in a number of studies; the data suggest thata more advanced educational level seems to be protec-tive against cognitive dysfunction, while poorer edu-cation was associated with a faster rate of cognitivedecline in nondemented patients with PD [2,6,11,15,17-19].

The aim of this study was to assess the relationshipbetween the level of education and cognitive perfor-mance in nondemented PD patients.

MMaatteerriiaall aanndd mmeetthhooddss

PPaattiieennttss

Thirty-seven consecutive PD patients admitted tothe Department of Neurology at Poznan University ofMedical Sciences were examined between January 2006and November 2007. Patients were included in the studyon the basis of UK Parkinson’s Disease Society BrainBank criteria for idiopathic PD and evaluated for theseverity of symptoms using the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS). None of the patientsfit the American Academy of Neurology (AAN) crite-ria for having dementia in PD assessed by the ClinicalDementia Rating (CDR) scale and the Mini-MentalState Examination (MMSE) [20,21]. Moreover, thesubjects with depression and advanced motor symptoms(UPDRS > 50) that impeded neuropsychologicalexamination were also excluded from the study.

The patients were treated with the following: levo -dopa plus a peripheral levodopa-decarboxylase inhibitor(37 patients), a dopamine agonist (9 patients), amanta-dine (11 patients), an anticholinergic drug (7 patients)and an inhibitor of monoamine oxidase B (5 patients).

Neurologic examinations were conducted in allpatients to confirm the diagnosis of PD. Additionally,magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) of the brain wasperformed for all patients. Those with severe ischaemicchanges, defined as 3 points (white matter and/or basalganglia) on the Age-Related White Matter ChangesScale, were excluded from the study [22].

CCoonnttrroollss

The control group consisted of 40 volunteers with-out any known neurological or vascular diseases, includ-ing cognitive impairment and depression.

To avoid including any subjects with detectableabnormalities of the nervous system all control persons

with a lower risk of cognitive deterioration. We conclude thathigher education might have protective effects in cognitivedecline in PD.

KKeeyy wwoorrddss:: Parkinson disease, cognitive impairment, educa-tional level.

i grupê kontroln¹ z wykszta³ceniem wy¿szym, nie stwierdzo-no znamiennych statystycznie ró¿nic dla wyników ¿adnegoz testów.WWnniioosskkii:: U osób z ChP bez otêpienia stwierdzono wystêpo-wanie niewielkiego stopnia, ³agodnych zaburzeñ poznawczych.Wy¿szy poziom wykszta³cenia zwi¹zany by³ z ni¿szym ryzy-kiem wyst¹pienia zaburzeñ, dlatego mo¿e byæ czynnikiemochronnym dla zaburzeñ poznawczych w ChP.

SS³³oowwaa kklluucczzoowwee:: choroba Parkinsona, zaburzenia poznaw-cze, poziom wykszta³cenia.

Educational level and cognitive impairment in PD patients

Page 3: Educational level and cognitive impairment in patients with Parkinson disease

NNeeuurroollooggiiaa ii NNeeuurroocchhiirruurrggiiaa PPoollsskkaa 2011; 45, 126

underwent the same neuropsychological assessmentprocess as PD patients.

EEtthhiiccaall rreeqquuiirreemmeennttss

Written informed consent was obtained from all sub-jects after the nature of the study was fully explained.The study was approved by the local Ethics Committeeof the University of Medical Sciences in Poznan, Poland.

NNeeuurrooppssyycchhoollooggiiccaall aasssseessssmmeenntt

We used the MMSE for the assessment of globalcognitive functioning, as well as the Hamilton Depres-sion Rating Scale (HDRS), to avoid any confoundingpresence of depression in the study [23]. The HDRSis recommended in screening for depression in PD, and,according to Schrag et al., the cut-off level was estab-lished at 13/14 points [24].

Specific neuropsychological tests were administeredfor the assessment of executive functions, includingattention and executive and visuospatial working mem-ory, as follows: • Trail Making Test (TMT), part A and B [25], Pol-

ish translation [26],

• Stroop Test part A (reading colour names in black)and B (naming the colour of different words; colour-word interference effect) [27], Polish translation [26],

• Mental Rotation Test (MRT) [28],• Verbal Fluency Test [29], for letters ‘k’, ‘m’, ‘p’

(an adaptation for Polish language according toDaniluk [30]).Overall, 12 cognitive measures were included in sta-

tistical analyses: time and numbers of errors in partsA and B of TMT, time and number of errors in bothparts of the Stroop test, number of correct responses inMRT (from 0 to 5), and number of words for the let-ters ‘k’, ‘m’ and ‘p’ in the Verbal Fluency Test in oneminute. Due to the lack of normative data for Polish ver-sions of tests assessing executive functions, the resultsof neuropsychological tests of PD patients were com-pared with the results of healthy controls. The groupswere matched for age and educational levels.

EEdduuccaattiioonn

The level of education of the patients and controls wasself-reported during the demographic interview and clas-sified for the purpose of this study by the years of educa-tion. Patients and controls were divided into 3 groups: low(primary or vocational education: 7-11 years of education),intermediate (secondary education: 12-13 years) or high-er educational level (university education: 14-17 years).

SSttaattiissttiiccaall aannaallyyssiiss

The distributions of continuous variables were eval-uated using the Kolmogorov and Smirnov method. Foreach variable with a normal distribution, we calculatedthe mean value and standard deviation (SD) and appliedunpaired t-tests.

Data with non-normal distributions are provided asgeometric mean and 1 SD range. The Mann-Whitneytest and Kruskal-Wallis H-test were used for these vari-ables to assess statistically significant differences betweenthe groups. The pre-established level of significance wastwo-tailed at p < 0.05.

RReessuullttss

SSttuuddyy ppooppuullaattiioonn

Thirty-seven subjects with PD and 40 controls wereenrolled for the study.

Clinical and demographic characteristics of theexamined subjects are shown in Table 1.

PPDD ppaattiieennttss** CCoonnttrroollss**nn == 3377 nn == 4400

Age, years 61.03 ± 7.5, 63.22 ± 6.4, mean ± SD, range 46-76 46-77

Gender, male/female, 15/22 26/14n (%) (40.5%/59.5%) (65%/35%)

Educational level, n (%)

low 10 (27.1%) 10 (25%)

intermediate 15 (40.5%) 17 (42.5%)

high 12 (32.4%0 13 (32.5%)

Age at disease onset, years 54.7 ± 6.9 –mean ± SD

UPDRS score 28.9 ± 12.4 –mean ± SD

Disease duration, years 6.2 ± 4.4 –mean ± SD

MMSE score 28.7 ± 1.3 29.2 ± 0.8mean ± SD

TTaabbllee 11.. Clinical and demographic characteristics of Parkinson disease (PD)patients and control subjects

SD – standard deviation

*PD patients and control groups did not differ significantly in terms of age, time of education

and MMSE score

Anna Kierzynka, Rados³aw KaŸmierski, Wojciech Kozubski

Page 4: Educational level and cognitive impairment in patients with Parkinson disease

NNeeuurroollooggiiaa ii NNeeuurroocchhiirruurrggiiaa PPoollsskkaa 2011; 45, 1 27

Educational level and cognitive impairment in PD patients

The PD and control groups did not differ signifi-cantly in terms of age, education and MMSE score.There were some disproportions in gender betweengroups; however, we did not find any differencesbetween males and females in terms of neuropsycho-logical tests within the education groups.

RReessuullttss ooff nneeuurrooppssyycchhoollooggiiccaall tteessttss iinn PPDD ppaattiieennttss

Significant differences were found on the basis ofeducational level for the majority of tests, with the excep-tion of the number of correct responses for the MRT.In relation to PD patients at the higher educational level, the poorer-educated (low and intermediate level)patients completed the tests more slowly and made moreerrors (Table 2).

RReessuullttss ooff nneeuurrooppssyycchhoollooggiiccaall tteessttss iinn ccoonnttrroollss

Significant differences were found in terms of edu-cational level (the lowest educational level was correlat-ed with poorer performance on 4 measures), time ofTMT A, time of TMT B, time of Stroop A, and num-bers of errors in TMT B (Table 3).

CCoommppaarriissoonnss bbeettwweeeenn PPDD ppaattiieennttss aanndd ccoonnttrroollssuubbjjeeccttss

Significant differences were found between PDpatients and controls. In comparison to healthy persons,the PD patients without dementia showed worse resultson neuropsychological tests that assessed executive functions for 10 evaluated measures. In addition, PD

TTeesstt EEdduuccaattiioonnaall lleevveell

LLooww IInntteerrmmeeddiiaattee HHiigghh

TMT A 9955..8800 6644..2266 4466..5500Mean time, sec. (SD) ((3322..7799)) ((2277..1100)) ((1166..7777))

TMT A 22..3300 00..4400 00..0000No. of errors, mean (SD) ((33..0066)) ((00..8833)) ((00..0000))

TMT B 225577..4400 115511..1133 110022..9911Mean time, sec. (SD) ((110055..1166)) ((7799..3377)) ((3355..5522))

TMT B 1111..4400 77..7733 22..7755 No. of errors, mean (SD) ((77..9955)) ((66..7799)) ((44..5577))

Verbal Fluency Test K, 88..9900 1111..0066 1144..0000n, mean (SD) ((22..3388)) ((22..6688)) ((33..4499))

Verbal Fluency Test M, 55..7700 99..6666 1111..2255n, mean (SD) ((22..6677)) ((22..9999)) ((44..0022))

Verbal Fluency Test P, 88..1100 88..8800 1122..4411n, mean (SD) ((33..5511)) ((33..5533)) ((44..2233))

MRT, No. of correct 3.10 3.00 3.67responses, mean (SD) (1.29) (0.65) (1.15)

STROOP A 4455..0000 3377..0000 2277..1177Mean time, sec. (SD) ((1133..8811)) ((1122..2288)) ((44..9955))

STROOP A 33..7700 33..2277 11..0000No. of errors, mean (SD) ((33..3377)) ((22..3311)) ((11..0044))

STROOP B 8866..9900 110044..8877 6677..0088Mean time, sec. (SD) ((2288..4444)) ((3388..4422)) ((1133..3300))

STROOP B 33..9900 00..4477 00..0088No. of errors, mean (SD) ((55..1133)) ((00..9922)) ((00..2299))

TTaabbllee 22.. Results of neuropsychological tests in Parkinson disease (PD)patients in different educational groups*

TMT – Trail Making Test, MRT – Mental Rotation Test

*Significant differences (p < 0.05) are shown in bold

TMT – Trail Making Test, MRT – Mental Rotation Test

*Significant differences (p < 0.05) are shown in bold

TTeesstt EEdduuccaattiioonnaall lleevveell

LLooww IInntteerrmmeeddiiaattee HHiigghh

TMT A 5566..7700 3355..0066 4444..8855Mean time, sec. (SD) ((1199..3333)) ((88..4455)) ((1177..4400))

TMT A 0.30 0.06 0.15No. of errors, mean (SD) (0.67) (0.24) (0.55)

TMT B 113300..0000 8844..7700 9988..3311Mean time, sec. (SD) ((4411..2288)) ((2233..4422)) ((2277..1122))

TMT B 44..2200 00..4477 11..6699No. of errors, mean (SD) ((55..2222)) ((11..2233)) ((22..6699))

Verbal Fluency Test K, 13.60 14.12` 14.23n, mean (SD) (4.15) (2.87) (3.06)

Verbal Fluency Test M, 9.70 10.53 11.77n, mean (SD) (3.74) (1.94) (2.80)

Verbal Fluency Test P, 10.00 11.88 13.69n, mean (SD) (4.08) (1.65) (3.64)

MRT, no. of correct 3.60 3.70 4.15responses, mean (SD) (1.35) (0.77) (0.99)

STROOP A 3333..4400 2255..5533 2255..3311Mean time, sec. (SD) ((99..2299)) ((33..4411)) ((33..9977))

STROOP A 1.10 0.59 0.38No. of errors, mean (SD) (1.29) (0.94) (0.77)

STROOP B 77.20 64.06 62.92Mean time, sec. (SD) (22.57) (9.29) (9.03)

STROOP B 0.40 0.06 0.15No. of errors, mean (SD) (0.69) (0.24) (0.55)

TTaabbllee 33.. Results of neuropsychological tests, in controls in different educa-tional groups*

Page 5: Educational level and cognitive impairment in patients with Parkinson disease

NNeeuurroollooggiiaa ii NNeeuurroocchhiirruurrggiiaa PPoollsskkaa 2011; 45, 128

patients’ responses were slower and exhibited moreerrors (Table 4). The less-educated (low and interme-diate level) PD patients, compared with less-educatedcontrols, displayed worse results for most of the psy-chological tests. In contrast to the lower educationgroups, there were no significant differences betweentest results of higher-educated PD patients and higher-educated controls (Table 5).

DDiissccuussssiioonn

Our study shows that PD patients who did not ful-fil standard criteria for dementia had specific cognitivedeficits in terms of visuospatial working memory, atten-tion and verbal fluency when compared to age- and edu-cation-matched controls without PD. Similar results

were obtained by other authors who compared patientsat earlier stages of PD with healthy subjects, in terms ofthe onset of cognitive disorders [1,4,7,9]. However,these studies found no significant differences betweengroups with respect to education. In our study, the analy-sis of the effect of education on cognitive performancefor the PD group demonstrated that higher-educatedsubjects performed best on most tests, and those withshorter education periods performed worse. Similareffects were also noticeable in subjects without PD.According to Spreen et al. [29], the poor education ofthe general, healthy population may particularly accountfor the results of the TMT and Stroop tests. In ourstudy, the time taken to complete the tests differed sig-nificantly, not only in PD patients but also in controlgroups. However, in the group of PD patients, the nega -tive influence of poorer education was clearly stronger.Significant differences between education groups areapparent for the results of most tests (11 of 12 measures)performed by PD patients. In the controls, significancewas achieved between education groups in only 4 out of12 measures (time TMT A, time TMT B, time StroopA, numbers of errors in TMT B) (Table 2).

Lower-educated PD patients (low and intermediateeducational level) performed worse on psychologicaltests compared with lower-educated controls (respec-tively, for 7 and 9 measures). The finding that there wereno significant differences in terms of test results betweenhigher-educated PD persons and higher-educated con-trols, however, suggests that more advanced educationmay foster a protective effect against early cognitivedecline in PD. The positive effect of a higher educa-tional level has also been described by Cohen et al. [17],who compared PD patients based on education level andreported better cognitive performance among higher-educated patients, especially for tests assessing execu-tive functions. A similar relationship was also affirmedby Kandiah et al. [2] in a de novo PD group, as well asby Green et al. [6] in patients with advanced PD. Inanother study targeting the lower educational level, moresevere disease states (as assessed by UPDRS total score)and older age at disease onset were the prominent riskfactors for dementia [18]. Additionally, in a meta-analy-sis that included 13 longitudinal studies, lower educa-tional levels have been found to be associated witha greater rate of cognitive decline in PD patients [19].From a theoretical point of view, better-educated per-sons may have greater cognitive reserves, which couldbe a protective factor against cognitive decline, especiallyin the early phases of the disease.

TTeesstt PPDD ppaattiieennttss CCoonnttrroollss

TMT A 6677..0033 ((3311..8844)) 4433..6655 ((1166..8888))Mean time, sec. (SD)

TMT A 0.78 (1.87) 0.15 (0.48)No. of errors, mean (SD)

TMT B 116644..2222 ((9966..6622)) 110000..4455 ((3344..3322))Mean time, sec. (SD)

TMT B 77..1111 ((77..1199)) 11..8800 ((33..3388))No. of errors, mean (SD)

verbal Fluency Test K, 1111..4433 ((33..4466)) 1144..0033 ((33..2211))N, mean, (SD)

verbal Fluency Test M, 99..1111 ((33..8899)) 1100..7733 ((22..8800))N, mean, (SD)

Verbal Fluency Test P, 99..7788 ((44..1111)) 1122..0000 ((33..3322))n, mean, (SD)

MRT, no. of correct 33..2244 ((11..0044)) 33..8833 ((11..0011))responses, mean (SD)

STROOP A 3355..9977 ((1122..7766)) 2277..4433 ((66..4466))Mean time, sec. (SD)

STROOP A 22..6655 ((22..5577)) 00..6655 ((11..0000))No. of errors, mean (SD)

STROOP B 8877..7766 ((3333..0099)) 6666..9988 ((1144..6633))Mean time, sec. (SD)

STROOP B 1.27 (3.10) 0.18 (0.50)No. of errors, mean (SD)

TTaabbllee 44.. Comparison of the results of neuropsychological tests between Parkinson disease (PD) patients and control subjects*

TMT – Trail Making Test, MRT – Mental Rotation Test

*Significant differences (p < 0.05) are shown in bold

Anna Kierzynka, Rados³aw KaŸmierski, Wojciech Kozubski

Page 6: Educational level and cognitive impairment in patients with Parkinson disease

NNeeuurroollooggiiaa ii NNeeuurroocchhiirruurrggiiaa PPoollsskkaa 2011; 45, 1 29

Cognitive and brain reserves are hypothetical constructsthat are suggested to occur in patients with Alzheimer disease [31,32]. However, the cognitive reserve theorymay, to some extent, help to explain the results of our study.According to Stern [31], cognitive reserve could be definedas ‘individual differences in how people process tasks, allow-ing some to cope better than others with brain pathology’.According to the same hypothesis, such a reserve theoryfocuses on the process that allows individuals to sustainbrain damage and maintain function. In the cognitivereserve construct, the are two main components: (a) neur-al reserve: assuming that brain networks are more efficientand have greater capacity or are more flexible in personswith greater reserve, greater neural reserve would equateto a larger capacity to cope with the disruption imposed bybrain pathology; and (b) neural compensation, which Stern

defined as ‘inter-individual variability in the ability to com-pensate for the pathological disruption of standard pro-cessing networks by using brain structures or networks notnormally used by individuals with an intact brain’ [31].Whether one, or both, of these mechanisms is involved inthe protective effect of education, and whether the mech-anism is influenced by working in more intellectuallydemanding professions, are still open questions.

Recently, it was shown that vascular risk factors, alongwith white matter abnormalities, probably do not con-tribute significantly to cognitive impairment in PDpatients, with the exception of PD patients with severecerebral ischaemic lesions [33,34]. Therefore, it is possi-ble that pathomechanisms of cognitive decline in PD couldbe different than in vascular dementia or Alzheimer dis-ease [34,35].

TTeesstt LLooww IInntteerrmmeeddiiaattee HHiigghheedduuccaattiioonnaall lleevveell eedduuccaattiioonnaall lleevveell eedduuccaattiioonnaall lleevveell

PPDD CCoonnttrroollss PPDD CCoonnttrroollss PPDD CCoonnttrroollssnn == 1100 nn == 1100 nn == 1155 nn == 1177 nn == 1122 nn == 1133

TMT A 9955..8800 ((3322..7799)) 5566..7700 ((1199..3333)) 6644..2277 ((2277..1100)) 3355..0066 ((88..4455)) 46.50 (16.77) 44.85 (17.40)Mean time, sec. (SD)

TMT A, 22..3300 ((33..0066)) 00..3300 ((00..6677)) 0.40 (0.83) 0.06 (0.24) 0.00 (0.00) 0.15 (0.55)No. of errors, mean (SD)

TMT B 225577..4400 ((110055..1166)) 113300..0000 ((4411..2233)) 115511..1133 ((7799..3377)) 8844..7711 ((2233..4422)) 102.92 (35.53) 98.31 (27.12)Mean time, sec. (SD)

TMT B 1111..4400 ((77..9955)) 44..2200 ((55..2222)) 77..7733 ((66..7799)) 00..4477 ((11..2233)) 2.75 (4.58) 1.69 (2.69)No. of errors, mean (SD)

Verbal Fluency Test K, 88..9900 ((22..3388)) 1133..6600 ((44..1144)) 1111..0077 ((22..6699)) 1144..1122 ((22..8877)) 14.00 (3.49) 14.23 (3.06)n, mean (SD)

Verbal Fluency Test M, 55..7700 ((22..6677)) 99..7700 ((33..7744)) 9.67 (2.99) 10.53 (1.94) 11.25 (4.03) 11.77 (2.80)n, mean (SD)

Verbal Fluency Test P, 8.10 (3.51) 10.00 (4.08) 88..8800 ((33..5533)) 1111..8888 ((11..6655)) 12.42 (4.23) 13.69 (3.64)n, mean (SD)

MRT, no. of correct 3.10 (1.29) 3.60 (1.35) 33..0000 ((00..6655)) 33..7711 ((00..7777)) 3.67 (1.15) 4.15 (0.99)responses, mean (SD)

STROOP A 4455..0000 ((1133..8822)) 3333..4400 ((99..3300)) 3377..0000 ((1122..2288)) 2255..5533 ((33..4411)) 27.17 (4.95) 25.31 (3.97)Mean time, sec. (SD)

STROOP A, 3.70 (3.37) 1.10 (1.29) 33..2277 ((22..3311)) 00..5599 ((00..9944)) 1.00 (1.04) 0.38 (0.77)No. of errors, mean (SD)

STROOP B 86.90 (28.44) 77.20 (22.57) 110044..8877 ((3388..4422)) 6644..0066 ((99..2299)) 67.08 (13.30) 62.92 (9.03)Mean time, sec. (SD)

STROOP B 3.90 (5.13) 0.40 (0.70) 0.47 (0.94) 0.06 (0.24) 0.08 (0.29) 0.15 (0.55)No. of errors, mean (SD)

TTaabbllee 55.. Comparison of educational level for matched groups of Parkinson disease (PD) patients and controls*

TMT – Trail Making Test, MRT – Mental Rotation Test

*Significant differences (p < 0.05) are shown in bold

Educational level and cognitive impairment in PD patients

Page 7: Educational level and cognitive impairment in patients with Parkinson disease

NNeeuurroollooggiiaa ii NNeeuurroocchhiirruurrggiiaa PPoollsskkaa 2011; 45, 130

CCoonncclluussiioonnss

1. It seems probable that educational achievements maypositively affect the cognitive performance after theonset of PD.

2. Hypothetically, protective effects of higher levels ofeducation, independent of dementia aetiology, may bedue to greater functional brain reserves.

DDiisscclloossuurree

Authors report no conflict of interest.

RReeffeerreenncceess

1. Aarsland D., Bro/ nnick K., Larsen J.P., et al. Cognitive impair-ment in incident, untreated Parkinson disease: the NorwegianParkWest study. Neurology 2009; 72: 1121-1126.

2. Kandiah N., Narasimhalu K., Lau P.N., et al. Cognitive declinein early Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 2009; 24: 605-608.

3. Mamikonyana E., Moberga P.J., Siderowf A., et al. Mild cog-nitive impairment is common in Parkinson’s disease patients withnormal Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores.Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2009; 15: 226-231.

4. Muslimovic D., Post B., Speelman J.D., et al. Cognitive profileof patients with newly diagnosed Parkinson disease. Neurology2005; 65: 1239-1245.

5. Caviness J.N., Driver-Dunckey E., Connor D.J., et al. Defin-ing mild cognitive impartment in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 2007; 22: 1272-1277.

6. Green J., McDonald W.M., Vitek J.L., et al. Cognitive impair-ments in advanced PD without dementia. Neurology 2002; 59:1320-1324.

7. Janvin C., Aarsland D., Larsen J.P., et al. Neuropsychologicalprofile of patients with Parkinson’s disease without dementia.Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2003; 15:126-131.

8. Taylor J.P., Rowan E.N., Lett D., et al. Poor attentional func-tion predicts cognitive decline in patients with non-dementedParkinson’s disease independent of motor phenotype. J NeurolNeurosurg Psychiatry 2008; 79: 1318-1323.

9. Verbaan D., Marinus J., Visser M., et al. Cognitive impairmentin Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2007; 78:1182-1187.

10. Aarsland D., Zaccai J., Brayne C. A systematic review of preva-lence studies of dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord2005; 20: 1255-1263.

11. Riedel O., Klotsche J., Spottke A., et al. Cognitive impairmentin 873 patients with idiopathic Parkinson’s disease. Results fromthe German Study on Epidemiology of Parkinson’s Disease withDementia (GEPAD). J Neurol 2008; 255: 255-264.

12. Buter T.C., van den Hout A., Matthews F.E., et al. Dementiaand survival in Parkinson disease: a 12-year population study.Neurology 2008; 70: 1017-1022.

13. Hely M., Reid W., Adena M., et al. The Sydney multicenterstudy of Parkinson’s disease: The inevitability of dementia at 20 years. Mov Disord 2008; 23: 837-844.

14. Aarsland D., Tandberg E., Larsen J.P., et al. Frequency ofdementia in Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol 1996; 53: 538-542.

15. Levy G., Schupf N., Tang M.X., et al. Combined effect of ageand severity on the risk of dementia in Parkinson’s disease. AnnNeurol 2002; 51: 722-729.

16. Burn D.J., Rowan E.N., Allan L.M., et al. Motor subtype andcognitive decline in Parkinson’s disease, Parkinson’s disease withdementia, and dementia with Lewy bodies. J Neurol NeurosurgPsychiatry 2006; 77: 585-589.

17. Cohen O.S., Vakil E., Tanne D., et al. Educational level as a mod-ulator of cognitive performance and neuropsychyatric features inParkinson disease. Cogn Behav Neurol 2007; 20: 68-72.

18. Glatt S.L., Hubble J.P., Lyons K., et al. Risk factors for demen-tia in Parkinson’s disease: effect of education. Neuroepidemiology1996; 15: 20-25.

19. Muslimoviæ D., Schmand B., Speelman J.D., et al. Course ofcognitive decline in Parkinson’s disease: a meta-analysis. J IntNeuropsychol Soc 2007; 13: 920-932.

20. Folstein M., Folstein S., McHugh P.R. Mini-Mental State:a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients forthe clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12: 189-198.

21. Miyasaki J.M., Shannon K., Voon V.K., et al. Quality StandardsSubcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. PracticeParameter: evaluation and treatment of depression, psychosis,and dementia in Parkinson disease (an evidence-based review):report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the AmericanAcademy of Neurology. Neurology 2006; 66: 996-1002.

22. Wahlund L.O., Barkhof F., Fazekas F., et al. A new rating scalefor age-related white matter changes applicable to MRI and CT.Stroke 2001; 32: 1318-1322.

23. Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol NeurosurgPsychiatry 1960; 23: 56-62.

24. Schrag A., Barone P., Brown R.G., et al. Depression rating scalesin Parkinson’s disease: critique and recommendations. Mov Disord 2007; 22: 1077-1092.

25. Reitan R.M. Validity of the trail making test as an indicator oforganic brain damage. Percept Mot Skills 1958; 8: 271-276.

26. Borkowska A., Sobów T. Ocena neuropsychologiczna w diag-nostyce i ró¿nicowaniu otêpienia czo³owo-skroniowego. NeurolNeurochir Pol 2005; 39: 466-475.

27. Stroop R.J. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol 1935; 18: 643-662.

28. http://www.sis.pitt.edu/~is1042/html/unit7.html29. Spreen O., Strauss E. A compendium of neuropsychological test.

Administration, norms and commentary. Oxford University Press,New York 2006.

30. Daniluk B. Deficyty poznawcze u osoby z postêpuj¹cym scho-rzeniem mózgu o etiologii naczyniowej. Neuropsychologicznestudium przypadku. In: Borkowska A., Szepietowska E. (eds.).Diagnoza neuropsychologiczna. Metodologia i metodyka.Wydawnictwo UMCS, Lublin 2000; pp. 383-405.

31. Stern Y. Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia 2009; 47: 2015-2028.

Anna Kierzynka, Rados³aw KaŸmierski, Wojciech Kozubski

Page 8: Educational level and cognitive impairment in patients with Parkinson disease

NNeeuurroollooggiiaa ii NNeeuurroocchhiirruurrggiiaa PPoollsskkaa 2011; 45, 1 31

32. Siedlecki K.L., Stern Y., Reuben A., et al. Construct validity ofcognitive reserve in multi-ethnic cohort: the Northern Manhat-tan Study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2009; 15: 558-569.

33. S³awek J., Wieczorek D., Derejko M., et al. Vascular risk factorsdo not contribute to motor and cognitive impairment in Parkin-son’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2010; 16: 73-74.

34. S³awek J., Wieczorek D., Derejko M., et al. The influence ofvascular risk factors and white matter hyperintensities on thedegree of cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. NeurolNeurochir Pol 2008; 42: 505-512.

35. Jellinger K.A. Prevalence of cerebrovascular lesions in Parkin-son’s disease. A postmortem study. Acta Neuropathol 2003; 105:415-419.

Educational level and cognitive impairment in PD patients